Chapter 2 References
Content from this module was adapted from the following openly licensed source:
OpenStax College. (2020). Psychology: Lifespan theories. https://openstax.org/books/psychology-2e/pages/9-2-lifespan-theories
Rio Salado College. (2020). Psychology and Culture PSY132. Riolearn. https://matrix.riosalado.edu/RioWebApps/Public/Rio.SyllabusDefinition.UI/MiniSyllabusRender.aspx?courseId=PSY132&specialtyCode=&instructionMode=IN&startDate=08-24-2020&termCode=4206§ionNum=41763
References
Bandura, A. (1995). Self-efficacy in changing societies. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
Bandura, A. (1977). Social learning theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Bandura, A. (1990). Some reflections on reflections. Psychological Inquiry, 1, 101-105.
Beall, C., Song, K., Elston, R., & Goldstein, M. (2004). Higher offspring survival among Tibetan women with high oxygen saturation genotypes residing at 4,000 m. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 101, 14300-4, doi: 10.1073/pnas.0405949101.
Bierema, L. & Merriam, S. (2014). Adult learning: Bridging theory and practice. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.
Boesch, C. & Tomasello, M. (1998). Chimpanzee and human cultures. Current Anthropology, 39, 591-614. https://doi.org/10.1086/204785
Bogartz, R. S., Shinskey, J. L., & Schilling, T. (2000). Object permanence in five-and-a-half-month-old infants? Infancy, 1(4), 403–428. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15327078IN0104_3
Bowles, D., Brown, A., Hughes, A., Morris, M., Sayers, P., Topple, A., Wallis, M., & Gardiner, K. (2013). Guide to risk assessment for reservoir safety management, Volume 1, 2. Deanery Road, Bristol, BS1 9AH: Environment Agency, Horizon House.
Boyd, R., Richerson, P. J., & Henrich, J. (2011). The cultural niche: Why social learning is essential for human adaptation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences Jun 2011, 108(Supplement 2) 10918-10925; doi: 10.1073/pnas.1100290108.
Call, J. & Tomasello, M. (1995). Use of social information in the problem solving of orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus) and human children (Homo sapiens). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 109, 308-320. https://doi.org/10.1037/0735-7036.109.3.308
Cone & Rand, D. G. (2014). Time pressure increases cooperation in competitively framed social dilemmas: a successful replication. PloS one, 9(12), 2014. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0115756
Creanza, N., Kolodny, O., & Feldman, M. W. (2017). How culture evolves and why it matters. National Academy of Science, 114(30), 7782-7789, doi: 10.1073/pnas.1620732114.
Derex, M., Beugin, M., Godelle, B., & Raymond, M. (2013). Experimental evidence for influence of group size on cultural complexity. Nature, 503(7476), 389–391. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature12774
Dunbar, R. I. M. (1993). Coevolution of neocortical size, group size and language in humans. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16(4): 681-694. doi:10.1017/S0140525X00032325
Fairlie, R., Hoffmann, F., & Oreopoulos, P. (2011). A community college instructor like me: Race and ethnicity interactions in the classroom. American Economic Review, 104, 10.1257/aer.104.8.2567. doi: 10.1257/aer.104.8.2567
Hamlin, J. K. & Wynn, K. (2011). Young infants prefer prosocial to antisocial others. Cognitive development, 26(1), 30–39. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2010.09.001.
Hamlin J. K., Wynn, K., Bloom, P. (2007). Social evaluation by preverbal infants. Nature, 2007; 450:557–559. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature06288
Hart, D. & Sussman. R. (2009). Man the hunted: Primates, predators, and human evolution, expanded edition. Boulder, CO: Westview Press.
Hauser, J. R., Urban, G. L., Liberali, G., & Braun, M. (2009). Website Morphing. Marketing Science, 28, 2, (March-April), 202-223. https://doi.org/10.1287/mksc.1080.0459
Heine, J. S. (2016). Cultural psychology. New York, NY: W. W. Norton & Company.
Henrich, J. (2004). Demography and cultural evolution: how adaptive cultural processes can produce maladaptive losses: the Tasmanian case. American Antiquity 69:197-214. https://doi.org/10.2307/4128416
Henrich, J. (2016). The secret of our success: How culture is driving human evolution, domesticating our species, and making us smarter. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
Herrmann, E., Call, J., Hernàndez-Lloreda, M. V., Hare, B., & Tomasello, M. (2007). Humans have evolved specialized skills of social cognition: The cultural intelligence hypothesis. Science, 317(5843), 1360–1366. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1146282
Hoffmann, F. & Oreopoulos, P. (2009). A professor like me: The influence of instructor gender on college achievement. Journal of Human Resources, 44(2), 479–494. https://doi.org/10.1353/jhr.2009.0024
McLeod, S. A. (2007). Behaviorism. Simply Psychology. Retrieved from http://www.simplypsychology.org/behaviorism.html.
McLeod, S. A. (2018). Jean Piaget’s theory of cognitive development. Simply Psychology. Retrieved from www.simplypsychology.org/piaget.html.
Melis, A. P. & Semmann, D. (2010). How is human cooperation different? Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 365, (1553), 2663–2674. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2010.0157
Piaget, J. (1936). Origins of intelligence in the child. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.
Rand, D. G. (2016). Cooperation, fast and slow: Meta-analytic evidence for a theory of social heuristics and self-interested deliberation. Psychological Science, 27(9), 1192–1206. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797616654455
Richerson, P. & Boyd, R. (2005). Not by genes alone: How culture transformed human evolution. Bibliovault OAI Repository, the University of Chicago Press, 10.7208/chicago/9780226712130.001.0001.
Skinner, B. F. (1953). Science and human behavior. New York, NY: Macmillan.
Tennie C., Call J., & Tomasello, M. (2009). Ratcheting up the ratchet: on the evolution of cumulative culture. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B. Biological Sciences, 364(1528), 2405–2415. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2009.0052
Tomasello, M. (1999). Emulation learning and cultural learning. Behavioural and Brain Sciences, 21, 703-704. doi: https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X98441748
Tomasello, M. & Carpenter, M. (2007). Shared intentionality. Developmental Science, 10(1), 121–125. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00573.x
Tomasello, M., Carpenter, M., Call J., Behne T., & Moll H. (2005). Understanding and sharing intentions: The origins of cultural cognition. Behavioral and Brain Sciences; 28(5):675–691. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X05000129
Tomasello, M., Kruger, A. C., & Ratner, H. H. (1993). Cultural learning. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 16(3), 495–552. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X0003123X.
Tomasello, M. (2016). Cultural learning redux. Child Development, 87(3), 643–653. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12499
Tomasello, M. & Moll, H. (2010). The gap is social: Human shared intentionality and culture. In P. M. Kappeler & J. B. Silk (Eds.), Mind the Gap: Tracing the Origins of Human Universals (pp. 331–349). Berlin, Germany: Springer.
Tomasello, M. & Rakoczy, H. (2003). What makes human cognition unique? From individual to shared to collective intentionality. Mind & Language, 18(2), 121–147. https://doi.org/10.1111/1468-0017.00217
Van de Vliert, E. (2011). Climato-economic origins of variation in ingroup favoritism. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 42(3), 494–515. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022022110381120.
Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in society: The development of higher psychological processes. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.